Meat consumption and risk of incident dementia: cohort study of 493,888 UK Biobank participants (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Meat consumption and risk of incident dementia: cohort study of 493,888 UK Biobank participants (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Am J Clin Nutr. 2021 Jul; 114(1): 175–184.

Published online 2021 Mar 22. doi:10.1093/ajcn/nqab028

PMCID: PMC8246598

PMID: 33748832

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

ABSTRACT

Background

Worldwide, the prevalence of dementia is increasing and diet as a modifiable factor could play a role. Meat consumption has been cross-sectionally associated with dementia risk, but specific amounts and types related to risk of incident dementia remain poorly understood.

Objective

We aimed to investigate associations between meat consumption and risk of incident dementia in the UK Biobank cohort.

Methods

Meat consumption was estimated using a short dietary questionnaire at recruitment and repeated 24-h dietary assessments. Incident all-cause dementia comprising Alzheimer disease (AD) and vascular dementia (VD) was identified by electronic linkages to hospital and mortality records. HRs for each meat type in relation to each dementia outcome were estimated in Cox proportional hazard models. Interactions between meat consumption and the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele were additionally explored.

Results

Among 493,888 participants included, 2896 incident cases of all-cause dementia, 1006 cases of AD, and 490 cases of VD were identified, with mean ± SD follow-up of 8 ±1.1 y. Each additional 25 g/day intake of processed meat was associated with increased risks of incident all-cause dementia (HR: 1.44; 95% CI: 1.24, 1.67; P-trend<0.001) and AD (HR: 1.52; 95% CI: 1.18, 1.96; P-trend=0.001). In contrast, a 50-g/d increment in unprocessed red meat intake was associated with reduced risks of all-cause dementia (HR: 0.81; 95% CI: 0.69, 0.95; P-trend=0.011) and AD (HR: 0.70; 95% CI: 0.53, 0.92; P-trend=0.009). The linear trend was not significant for unprocessed poultry and total meat. Regarding incident VD, there were no statistically significant linear trends identified, although for processed meat, higher consumption categories were associated with increased risks. The APOE ε4 allele increased dementia risk by 3 to 6 times but did not modify the associations with diet significantly.

Conclusion

These findings highlight processed-meat consumption as a potential risk factor for incident dementia, independent of the APOE ε4 allele.

Keywords: dementia, Alzheimer disease, vascular dementia, meat consumption, processed meat, UK Biobank

See corresponding editorial on page 7 and article on page 154.

Introduction

Dementia is a major public health concern with around 50 million cases globally and an incidence of nearly 10 million new cases per annum (1, 2). It comprises Alzheimer disease (AD), which contributes to 50–70% of dementia cases, vascular dementia (VD), which contributes to ∼25%, and other forms of dementia (2, 3). Dementia development and progression are associated with both genetic and environmental factors, including diet and lifestyle (4, 5). Lifestyle-related and dietary factors associated with dementia are potentially modifiable and thus represent targets for primary prevention (6).

Meat consumption has gained increasing interest in relation to health, since high consumption of processed meat and probably red meat were found to be consistently associated with an increased risk of colorectal cancer (7). In recent decades meat consumption has doubled or even tripled globally, especially in developing countries (8). This dietary transition has been associated with increasing AD prevalence in Japan, Peru, Cuba and other low- and middle-income countries in both ecological and cross-sectional studies (9, 10). A study of cognitively healthy individuals in Sweden showed that low consumption of meat and meat products was associated with better cognitive performance in clinical dementia screening tests and greater total brain volume after a 5-y follow-up period (11). Our previous review on meat consumption and cognitive disorders including dementia showed that most meat-related studies were embedded in complex dietary patterns with considerable heterogeneity, and the evidence of associations between risk of dementia and specific types or amounts of meat consumption was limited (12).

A consistent association has been established between carriage of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele and elevated risk of dementia or AD (13). Previous stratified analyses by APOE ε4 status showed that unfavourable lifestyle factors (e.g., less healthy dietary pattern, less physical activity, smoking, and social isolation) were associated with higher risk of dementia in APOE ε4 noncarriers but not in carriers (14). The discrepancy between carriers and noncarriers indicates that APOE genotype may modify associations between lifestyle factors and dementia risks, and might be explained by a potential masking of weak associations from lifestyle factors by the strongly associated APOE ε4 allele. However, at present whether APOE ε4 allele carriage interacts with lifestyle factors, such as diet, influencing risk of dementia remains unclear.

In the present study we examined the hypothesis that high consumption of meat increases the incidence of dementia in the general population, which may be more pronounced among APOE ε4 noncarriers.

Methods

Study design

The UK Biobank is a largescale population-based cohort study of half a million participants aged 40–69 y recruited from across the United Kingdom between 2006 and 2010 (15). The Biobank recruited participants using National Health Service patient registers and conducted the baseline assessments across 22 assessment centers in England, Scotland, and Wales which included a touchscreen questionnaire, verbal interview, physical measures, and biosample collection. At recruitment, participants electronically signed consent forms and completed various touchscreen questionnaires and measurements. All available resources are listed on the UK Biobank website (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/resources/). Ethical approval was granted for the UK Biobank by the North West–Haydock Research Ethics Committee (REC reference: 16/NW/0274). The UK Biobank dataset for this project included 502,493 participants.

Meat consumption measures

At the recruitment assessment-center visit, each participant was asked to complete a brief touchscreen FFQ with 47 dietary items covering main foods, food groups, and drinking habits (16). The meat-related questionnaire items (fish not included) examined in the current study were the following: processed meat (such as bacon, ham, sausages, meat pies, kebabs, burgers, chicken nuggets), unprocessed poultry, unprocessed beef, unprocessed lamb/mutton, and unprocessed pork. Consumption of unprocessed beef, lamb, and pork were summed to provide the “unprocessed red meat” type, and all meat items listed above were combined into “total meat.” Frequencies of consumption consisted of 6 categories and were assigned values for frequency per week (never eaten=0, eaten <1 time/wk=0.5, 1 time/wk=1, 2–4 times/wk=3, 5–6 times/wk=5.5, and ≥1 time daily=7). We categorized intake frequencies for each meat type into 5 groups as follows: processed meat (0, 0.1–0.9, once, 2.0–4.9, and≥5.0 times/wk), unprocessed poultry (0, 0.1–0.9, once, 2.0–4.9, and≥5.0 times/wk), unprocessed red meat (0, 0.1–1.0, 1.1–1.9, 2.0–2.9, and≥3.0 times/wk), and total meat (0, 0.1–3.0, 3.1–4.9, 5.0–6.9, and≥7.0 times/wk). These categories were determined based on data distribution to provide similar-sized groups (additional details in Supplemental Methods 1).

As an enhancement to the baseline touchscreen brief FFQ, the Oxford WebQ dietary questionnaire (17), which assesses a more detailed dietary intake over the previous 24 h was added to the assessment centers from April 2009 to September 2010. After that the WebQ questionnaire was administered online once every 3–4 mo and repeated for a total of 4 rounds over a 16-mo period from February 2011 to June 2012 for 24-h dietary assessments. The Oxford WebQ asked participants to select the number of portions for each item they consumed over the previous 24-h period with instructions specifying 1 standard portion size such as 1 sausage, 1 rasher of bacon, or 1 serving of beef. The daily intakes in grams were calculated by multiplying reported numbers of portions by standard portion sizes (16). Similar foods were then combined together into distinct meat types to match the baseline touchscreen questionnaire. A subgroup of participants (n  = 126,844) who completed at least two 24-h dietary assessments were included in this study (18) (see comparisons between participants without or with 1+, 2+, and 3+ completions of the Oxford WebQ in Supplemental Table 1); values from multiple assessments were averaged for each participant with 2+ completions. We then calculated the mean intakes from the 24-h dietary assessments within each category of meat types from the touchscreen brief FFQ. The corresponding mean daily intakes in each category were used in combination with frequency from the touchscreen questionnaire as continuous variables to examine the effect sizes per specific increment of meat intakes (25 g/d for processed meat and unprocessed poultry; 50 g/d for unprocessed red meat and total meat). These increments correspond to usual average portion sizes for regular eaters of these products, especially in men in the UK Biobank (19), and are consistent with other study presentations of results (20). The mean daily intakes in each meat category were also used to test the P-trend across 5 categories of each meat type, as well as to correct for the potential regression dilution bias in the touchscreen brief FFQ reported in previous studies (16, 21) (more details seen in Supplemental Methods 1).

Ascertainment of dementia

Prevalent and incident dementia cases within the UK Biobank were ascertained through data linkage to hospital inpatient admissions and death registries. Self-reported dementia cases at recruitment were additionally classified as prevalent cases. The electronic linkage to hospital inpatient data and death registry records includes primary or secondary events across healthcare systems in England, Scotland, and Wales. Date of diagnosis was set as the earliest date of dementia codes recorded regardless of source used. According to the International Classification of Diseases (ICD), AD was defined as code 331.0 in edition 9 and codes F00 and G30 in edition 10; VD was defined as codes 290.4 in edition 9 and codes F01 and I67.3 in edition 10; all-cause dementia was defined as all of the above codes plus ICD-9 codes 290, 291.2, 294.1, 331.0–331.2, and 331.5, and ICD-10 codes A81.0, F02, F05.1, F10.6, G31.0, G31.1, and G31.8. The updating date of linkages to hospital inpatient admission and death registries was 31 March 2017 in England, 31 October 2016 in Scotland, and 29 February 2016 in Wales in this study. Participant survival time in person-y was calculated from the date of dietary assessment until date of dementia diagnosis, date of loss to follow-up, date of death, or updating date of linkages.

APOE genotyping

Genotypes of nearly one-half million participants in the UK Biobank were assayed using 2 very similar genotyping arrays manufactured by Affymetrix: the BiLEVE Axiom array for ∼50,000 participants and the UK Biobank Axiom array for the remaining ∼450,000 participants; genotyping quality control was performed by UK Biobank centrally (22). Data from UK Biobank participants with unusually high heterozygosity and missingness (>5%) and disagreement between reported sex and genetic sex were excluded in genotype-related analyses (23). In addition, we used genetic kinship to other participants (Biobank field ID 22,021) as a covariate to limit confounding from population relatedness (24). The APOEhaplotypes (ε2/ε3/ε4) were directly genotyped and determined by 2 genetic variants, rs429358 and rs7412. Participants with 1 or 2 ε4 alleles were defined as APOE ε4 carriers and otherwise as APOE ε4 noncarriers. After quality control procedures, APOE genotypes were available on 405,126 UK Biobank participants and were included in APOE genotype related analyses.

Statistical analysis

Participants with prevalent dementia, and those with incomplete data on meat-related variables were excluded before analyses. Given the possibility that underlying dementia may cause changes in dietary behaviors in advance of diagnosis, we excluded incident dementia cases that occurred in the first-y period from baseline dietary data collection to dementia diagnosis to limit the possibility of reverse causality (25). A more stringent 3-y cut-off was also applied as a sensitivity analysis (see the flowchart in Supplemental Figure 1).

Baseline sociodemographic, lifestyle, and main dietary characteristics were summarized and stratified by dementia status (incident dementia and no dementia). Among incident cases, all-cause dementia, AD, and VD were treated as separate outcomes. The associations between incident dementia and reported consumption of processed meat, unprocessed poultry, unprocessed red meat, and total meat were fitted in Cox proportional hazards regressions with the duration of follow-up in years as the timescale and the second lowest category of meat intakes as the reference; HRs with 95% CIs were reported for all analyses.

Three models were applied in our analyses: unadjusted models, minimally adjusted models, and fully adjusted models. The minimally adjusted model was adjusted for age at baseline, gender, self-reported ethnicity (White, Asian, Black, mixed, other/unknown), socioeconomic status (low, moderate, or high deprivation), educational level (with university/college degree or not), determined by a directed acyclic graph (26) (Supplemental Methods 2). The fully adjusted model was additionally adjusted for region (England, Wales, Scotland), BMI (in kg/m2; <25, 25–29.9, and ≥30), physical activity level (low, moderate, and high), smoking status (never, past, and current), typical sleep duration (<7, 7–8, >8 h/d), stroke history, family history of dementia, and dietary factors including total consumption of vegetables and fruits, total fish, tea and coffee, and alcohol. Processed meat, unprocessed poultry, and unprocessed red meat were also mutually adjusted for in the models. More details on covariates can be seen in Supplemental Methods 3. For covariates where participants answered “do not know” or “prefer not to answer,” these responses were classified as missing. An “unknown” category was created to replace missing values for each covariate; the effect of replacement of missing values was assessed by a sensitivity analysis conducted in participants with complete data on all covariates.

To investigate potential modifying effects of the APOE ε4 allele on risk of dementia from meat consumption, stratified analyses by APOE ε4 carrying status were conducted and additionally P-interaction between each meat type and APOE ε4 status was tested. As a sensitivity analysis, the main analyses were repeated among participants aged ≥60 y at baseline since individuals aged >60 y have a higher risk of incident dementia (27). Statistical analyses were conducted using Stata/IC, version 16.1 (Stata Corp LP).

Results

During a mean follow-up of 8 ± 1.1 y, excluding cases arising in the first year of follow-up (n=77), 2896 incident cases of all-cause dementia occurred, of which 1006 were AD and 490 were VD. Baseline characteristics stratified by dementia status are provided inTable 1. Dementia cases were generally older, more economically deprived, less educated, more likely to smoke, less physically active, more likely to have stroke history and family dementia history, and more likely to be APOE ε4 carriers. More men than women were diagnosed with dementia in the study population. Participant characteristics across 5 categories of reported consumption of processed meat, unprocessed poultry, unprocessed red meat, and total meat are shown in Supplemental Tables 2, 3, 4, and 5 respectively. Generally, compared with those in the lowest category, participants in higher categories of reported consumption of processed meat and total meat were more likely to be men, less educated, smokers, and overweight or obese, and had lower intakes of vegetables and fruits and higher intakes of energy, protein, and fat (including saturated fat).

TABLE 1

Baseline characteristics of participants stratified by dementia status in the UK Biobank cohort study1

All participants (n  =  493,888)Incident dementia (n=2896)No dementia (n=490,992)
Age at baseline, y56.5 ± 8.163.7 ± 5.556.5 ± 8.1
Duration of follow-up, y8.0 ± 1.15.9 ± 2.18.0 ± 1.1
Gender
 Men224,691 (45.5%)1625 (56.1%)223,066 (45.4%)
 Women269,197 (54.5%)1271 (43.9%)267,926 (54.6%)
Ethnicity
 White466,835 (94.5%)2757 (95.2%)464,078 (94.5%)
 Asian10,737 (2.2%)44 (1.5%)10,693 (2.2%)
 Black7454 (1.5%)52 (1.8%)7402 (1.5%)
 Mixed2951 (0.6%)13 (0.4%)2938 (0.6%)
 Others/unknown5911 (1.2%)30 (1.0%)5881 (1.2%)
Region
 England438,178 (88.7%)2510 (86.7%)435,668 (88.7%)
 Wales20,505 (4.2%)121 (4.2%)20,384 (4.2%)
 Scotland35,205 (7.1%)265 (9.2%)34,940 (7.1%)
Townsend deprivation index
 Low deprivation164,443 (33.3%)858 (29.6%)163,585 (33.3%)
 Moderate deprivation164,409 (33.3%)876 (30.2%)163,533 (33.3%)
 High deprivation164,426 (33.3%)1160 (40.1%)163,266 (33.3%)
 Unknown610 (0.1%)2 (0.1%)608 (0.1%)
Educational level
 Without college/university degree327,638 (66.3%)2245 (77.5%)325,393 (66.3%)
 With college/university degree161,496 (32.7%)582 (20.1%)160,914 (32.8%)
 Unknown4754 (1.0%)69 (2.4%)4685 (1.0%)
Smoking status
 Never269,599 (54.6%)1273 (44.0%)268,326 (54.6%)
 Past170,941 (34.6%)1233 (42.6%)169,708 (34.6%)
 Current51,734 (10.5%)371 (12.8%)51,363 (10.5%)
 Unknown1614 (0.3%)19 (0.7%)1595 (0.3%)
Physical activity level
 Low75,335 (15.3%)478 (16.5%)74,857 (15.2%)
 Moderate162,588 (32.9%)882 (30.5%)161,706 (32.9%)
 High160,784 (32.6%)779 (26.9%)160,005 (32.6%)
 Unknown95,181 (19.3%)757 (26.1%)94,424 (19.2%)
BMI, kg/m2
 Normal/underweight (<25)162,906 (33.0%)893 (30.8%)162,013 (33.0%)
 Overweight (25–29.9)208,812 (42.3%)1184 (40.9%)207,628 (42.3%)
 Obese (≥30)119,702 (24.2%)775 (26.8%)118,927 (24.2%)
 Unknown2468 (0.5%)44 (1.5%)2424 (0.5%)
Sleep duration
 <7 h/d120,987 (24.5%)750 (25.9%)120,237 (24.5%)
 7–8 h/d332,852 (67.4%)1687 (58.3%)331,165 (67.4%)
 >8 h/d37,564 (7.6%)415 (14.3%)37,149 (7.6%)
 Unknown2485 (0.5%)44 (1.5%)2441 (0.5%)
With stroke history7397 (1.5%)177 (6.1%)7220 (1.5%)
With family history of dementia57,728 (11.7%)558 (19.3%)57,170 (11.6%)
APOE ε4 carrying status
 Noncarriers290,382 (58.8%)1177 (40.6%)289,205 (58.9%)
 Carriers115,873 (23.5%)1182 (40.8%)114,691 (23.4%)
 Missing87,633 (17.7%)537 (18.5%)87,096 (17.7%)
Total meat
 Never20,473 (4.1%)94 (3.2%)20,379 (4.2%)
 ≤3 times/wk77,261 (15.6%)459 (15.8%)76,802 (15.6%)
 3–5 times/wk90,065 (18.2%)509 (17.6%)89,556 (18.2%)
 ≥5 times/wk162,570 (32.9%)875 (30.2%)161,695 (32.9%)
 ≥7 times/wk143,519 (29.1%)959 (33.1%)142,560 (29.0%)
Vegetables/fruits
 <2 servings/d28,960 (5.9%)194 (6.7%)28,766 (5.9%)
 <4 servings/d133,350 (27.0%)638 (22.0%)132,712 (27.0%)
 4–6 servings/d190,853 (38.6%)1032 (35.6%)189,821 (38.7%)
 >6 servings/d128,487 (26.0%)893 (30.8%)127,594 (26.0%)
 Unknown12,238 (2.5%)139 (4.8%)12,099 (2.5%)
Total fish
 ≤1 times/wk126,980 (25.7%)678 (23.4%)126,302 (25.7%)
 1–2 times/wk107,219 (21.7%)520 (18.0%)106,699 (21.7%)
 ≥2 times/wk150,200 (30.4%)865 (29.9%)149,335 (30.4%)
 ≥4 times/wk106,331 (21.5%)791 (27.3%)105,540 (21.5%)
 Unknown3158 (0.6%)42 (1.5%)3116 (0.6%)
Alcohol
 <1 time/wk150,575 (30.5%)1075 (37.1%)149,500 (30.4%)
 1–2 times/wk127,529 (25.8%)664 (22.9%)126,865 (25.8%)
 3–4 times/wk114,501 (23.2%)536 (18.5%)113,965 (23.2%)
 Daily or almost daily100,944 (20.4%)610 (21.1%)100,334 (20.4%)
 Unknown339 (0.1%)11 (0.4%)328 (0.1%)
Tea/coffee
 ≤3 cups/d108,836 (22.0%)663 (22.9%)108,173 (22.0%)
 ≤5 cups/d161,965 (32.8%)918 (31.7%)161,047 (32.8%)
 ≤7 cups/d132,660 (26.9%)698 (24.1%)131,962 (26.9%)
 >7 cups/d88,987 (18.0%)593 (20.5%)88,394 (18.0%)
 Unknown1440 (0.3%)24 (0.8%)1416 (0.3%)

1Continues variables displayed as means ± SDs, and categorical variables are displayed as numbers (percentages). APOE, apolipoprotein E.

The associations between each meat type and each dementia outcome were analyzed in three adjustment models. For the incident all-cause dementia (Figure 1), there was a significant linear trend for each additional 25 g processed meat consumed per day (HR: 1.44; 95% CI: 1.24, 1.67; P-trend<0.001). Unprocessed red meat appeared to be protective, with a HR of 0.81 for each additional 50 g intake per day (95% CI: 0.69, 0.95; P-trend=0.011) in the fully adjusted model. The linear-trend was not statistically significant for unprocessed poultry in relation to risk of all-cause dementia. For total meat, there was a borderline increased risk of incident all-cause dementia (HR: 1.09; 95% CI: 1.00, 1.19; P-trend=0.057).

Meat consumption and risk of incident dementia: cohort study of 493,888 UK Biobank participants (2)

HRs (95% CIs) for the associations between incident all-cause dementia and meat consumption in UK Biobank (n=493,888). The black squares and horizontal lines represent HRs and 95% CIs respectively in Cox proportional-hazards regressions. The distribution of ticks on the x axis is exponential. Participants were categorized based on the data distribution of baseline meat intakes. Mean daily intakes in each category were calculated from the multiple 24-h dietary assessments which were used to test the linear trend per increment. Minimally adjusted models adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, education, socioeconomic status. Fully adjusted models additionally adjusted for region, smoking status, physical activity, BMI, sleep duration, stroke history, and family history of dementia, and dietary covariates including vegetables and fruits, total fish, tea and coffee, alcohol drinking, processed meat, unprocessed poultry, and unprocessed red meat were also mutually adjusted for.

In terms of incident AD (Figure 2), a similar picture to all-cause dementia was seen. Higher consumption of processed meat was associated with increased risk of AD (HR: 1.52 per additional 25 g/d; 95% CI: 1.18, 1.96; P-trend=0.001). Higher consumption of unprocessed red meat was associated with reduced risk of AD (HR: 0.70 per additional 50 g/d; 95% CI: 0.53, 0.92; P-trend=0.009). Regarding the risk of incident VD (Figure 3), there were no statistically significant linear trends identified, although for processed meat, the highest consumption categories were associated with increased risk. For all dementia outcomes, 0 times/wk consumption of each meat type appeared to be different from other higher frequencies (Figure1,2, and3); however, most HRs in this category were not significant in the fully adjusted models.

Meat consumption and risk of incident dementia: cohort study of 493,888 UK Biobank participants (3)

HRs (95% CIs) for the associations between incident Alzheimer disease and meat consumption in UK Biobank (n=493,888). The black squares and horizontal lines represent HRs and 95% CI, respectively, in Cox proportional-hazards regressions. The distribution of ticks on the x axis is exponential. Participants were categorized based on the data distribution of baseline meat intakes. Mean daily intakes in each category is calculated from the multiple 24-h dietary assessments which were used to test the linear trend per increment. Minimally adjusted models adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, education, and socioeconomic status. Fully adjusted models additionally adjusted for region, smoking status, physical activity, BMI, sleep duration, stroke history, family history of dementia, and dietary covariates including vegetables and fruits, total fish, tea and coffee, alcohol drinking; processed meat, unprocessed poultry, and unprocessed red meat were also mutually adjusted for.

Meat consumption and risk of incident dementia: cohort study of 493,888 UK Biobank participants (4)

HRs (95% CIs) for the associations between incident vascular dementia and meat consumption in UK Biobank (n=493,888). The black squares and horizontal lines represent HRs and 95% CI respectively in Cox proportional-hazards regressions. The distribution of ticks on the x axis is exponential. Participants were categorized based on the data distribution of baseline meat intakes. Mean daily intakes in each category is calculated from the multiple 24-h dietary assessments which were used to test the linear trend per increment. Minimally adjusted models adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, education, socioeconomic status. Fully adjusted models additionally adjusted for region, smoking status, physical activity, BMI, sleep duration, stroke history, and family history of dementia, and dietary covariates including vegetables and fruits, total fish, tea and coffee, alcohol drinking; processed meat, unprocessed poultry, and unprocessed red meat were also mutually adjusted for.

The stratified analyses by APOE ε4 carrying status and P values for interaction between APOE ε4 carriage and meat consumption are shown inTable 2 and Supplemental Table 6. Compared with APOE ε4 noncarriers, carriers had increased risks of developing all-cause dementia by ∼3 times, AD by ∼6 times, and VD by ∼5 times, independent of any type of meat consumption. However, there were no statistically significant interactions between APOE ε4 carriage and meat consumption in the fully adjusted models. Increased risks of incident all-cause dementia were observed per 25 g/d increments of processed meat in both APOE ε4 carriers and noncarriers. However, APOE ε4 carriers but not noncarriers had reduced risks of incident all-cause dementia and incident AD per 50 g/d increment of unprocessed red meat.

TABLE 2

Risks of all-cause dementia under different meat types among APOE Ɛ4 noncarriers (n=289,589) and carriers ( n=115,537) respectively 1

Unadjusted models (n=405,126)Minimally adjusted models (n=405,126)Fully adjusted models (n=405,126)
HRLCIUCIPHRLCIUCIPHRLCIUCIP
Risk of all-cause dementia
APOE Ɛ4 carriers vs. noncarriers3.312.384.61<0.0013.592.485.19<0.0013.512.445.04<0.001
Processed meat, 25 g/d
 Stratified analysis
 APOE Ɛ4 noncarriers1.641.332.02<0.0011.361.091.700.0071.461.151.840.002
 APOE Ɛ4 carriers1.180.961.450.1121.090.881.360.4361.471.161.850.001
P for interaction with APOE Ɛ4 allele0.0270.0260.185
Unprocessed poultry, 25 g/d
 Stratified analysis
 APOE Ɛ4 noncarriers0.840.740.960.0090.920.791.070.2610.930.791.090.379
  APOE Ɛ4 carriers0.820.730.930.0020.890.771.030.1110.940.811.090.435
P-interaction with APOE Ɛ4 allele0.7870.3290.765
Unprocessed red meat, 50 g/d
 Stratified analysis
 APOE Ɛ4 noncarriers1.311.041.660.0230.940.751.190.6330.930.721.210.594
  APOE Ɛ4 carriers0.890.721.110.3110.640.510.80<0.0010.640.500.82<0.001
P-interaction with APOE Ɛ4 allele0.0200.0190.095
Total meat, 50 g/d
 Stratified analysis
  APOE Ɛ4 noncarriers1.221.071.390.0031.110.961.280.1681.161.001.340.044
  APOE Ɛ4 carriers1.050.931.170.4620.950.831.090.4691.020.891.170.816
P-interaction with APOE Ɛ4 allele0.0910.0620.054

1Minimally adjusted models: Cox proportional-hazards regression adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, education, socioeconomic status. Fully adjusted models: Cox proportional-hazards regression additionally adjusted for region, smoking status, physical activity, BMI, sleep duration, stroke history, family history of dementia, genetic kinship to other participants, dietary covariates including vegetables and fruits, total fish, tea and coffee, alcohol drinking, processed meat, unprocessed poultry, and unprocessed red meat were also mutually adjusted for. Mean daily intakes per increment calculated from the multiple 24-h dietary assessments were used as continuous variables in Cox models. APOE, apolipoprotein E; LCI, lower CI (95%); UCI, upper CI (95%).

When we additionally excluded dementia cases occurring within the first 3-y follow-up (n=329) for more rigorous controls of potential reverse causality, the HRs were of similar magnitude (Supplemental Figure 2, 3, and 4, Supplemental Table 7). When we conducted a sensitivity analysis in participants with complete data on all covariates (n=381,809), the HRs were very similar to the main results (Supplemental Figure 5, 6, and 7, Supplemental Table 8). Exclusion of participants aged <60 y at baseline also did not significantly change these associations (Supplemental Figure 8, 9, and 10, Supplemental Table 9).

Discussion

In this population-based, nationwide UK Biobank cohort study our results showed that consumption of processed meat was associated with increased risks of incident all-cause dementia and AD while unprocessed red meat was associated with lower risks. Related cohort studies remain few and inconsistent, and detailed knowledge of which type and amount of meat consumption would be the most influential is not clear. The Three-City (3C) cohort study took meat consumption of high frequency (≥4 times/wk) as the reference and found that low frequency (≤1 times/wk) was related to an increased risk of incident dementia and AD over 10 y of follow-up (28), which is inconsistent with our findings; however, the methods of collapsing data and reference selection are different. In addition, excessive category combination may have attenuated the study power and specific meat types were not explored in that study. A cohort study conducted in French citizens aged 68 and over showed that compared with daily meat consumers, weekly or less consumers had a higher incidence rate of all-cause dementia and AD after 7 y of follow-up; however, those associations were not significant probably because of small sample sizes (170 incident dementia including 135 AD among 1674 participants) (29). Longitudinal analysis among 2622 elderly German participants suggested no significant association between risk of incident AD and consumption frequency of meat and sausage after 4 y of follow-up (30); however, this study only investigated single meat items.

Our results also showed that presence of the APOE ε4 allele increased the risk of incident dementia, especially AD; however, there were only minor differences in associations between meat consumption and dementia risk among APOE ε4 noncarriers and carriers, and all P values for interaction were nonsignificant. Currently, evidence on the interaction between APOE genotype and dietary factors with dementia has mostly focused on dietary patterns and dietary fat intake; those studies found older individuals (aged≥60 y) who had a diet high in fatty fish or higher polyunsaturated fat intake were associated with a decreased risk of all-cause dementia, especially among APOE ε4 noncarriers (31, 32). In contrast, studies conducted at midlife found that moderate to high intake of saturated fats in relation to an increased risk of dementia/AD was only detected or more pronounced among APOE ε4 carriers (33, 34). A German cohort study of individuals aged 75+found there was no difference in the association of meat and sausage consumption with incident AD risk between APOE ε4 noncarriers and carriers (30). In addition, a cohort study from eastern Finland showed that the APOE ε4 genotype did not modify associations of egg and cholesterol intakes with risk of incident dementia and AD over ∼22 y of follow-up (35). Inconsistency in these and our study results may reflect particular cohort characteristics; in particular our participants were younger (50–68 y) and this may have led to our insignificant interactions between APOE genotype and meat intake with dementia risk in this population. It is also possible that APOE ε4 carriage is an independent process from dietary aspects in relation to dementia risk.

The underlying reasons for the inconsistent associations between different meat types in relation to dementia risk are not understood. High levels of protein in meat may potentially explain the link between unprocessed meat intake and a lower risk of dementia; adequate protein intake has been linked to a reduced risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in the elderly (36). High iron levels in unprocessed red meat may be protective, with iron deficiency being associated with decreased cognitive and attentional processes. Studies in animals have shown a negative impact of iron deficiency on myelination (37). On the other hand, as people age, iron deposits in the brain may impair normal cognitive function. Abnormal iron metabolism triggers oxidative stress, a major contributor to neurodegeneration (38). Processed meat contains nitrites and N-nitroso compounds, which may result in oxidative stress, lipid peroxidation, and activation of proinflammatory cytokines or other mechanisms potentially involved in the development of dementia (39). In addition, as meat consumption increases, intake of saturated fatty acids increases, which has been associated with a higher risk of dementia (40). Processed meat is often high in sodium, and rats fed a long-term high-salt diet had a marked increase in systolic blood pressure linked to reduced regional cerebral blood flow, and potentially linked to cognitive deficit (41). These differences in nutritional composition may explain why consumption of processed meat was associated with a higher risk of dementia rather than unprocessed poultry and unprocessed red meat. These potentially beneficial and negative effects of different meat types on risk of dementia may exist simultaneously, leading to the inconsistent associations seen with meat in this study.

A major strength of the current study is that the prospective study with large sample sizes ensured sufficient statistical power. To our knowledge, this is the first study to estimate specific meat types in relation to several dementia outcomes with additional exploration of interactions with the APOE ε4 allele. Other strengths include use of multiple data linkages to maximize capture of incident dementia outcomes, and consideration of reverse causation in analyses. Nevertheless, our study has several limitations. Firstly, the baseline touchscreen brief FFQ only covered some commonly consumed foods and was not suitable to assess total energy or nutrient intakes; systematic bias from self-reported measures at recruitment and low responses to the more detailed repeated 24-h dietary assessments with less than half participants may limit generalizability. Secondly, the UK Biobank cohort study does not have a long follow-up (∼8 y). This will limit our ability to distinguish between reverse causation and causality for risk factors for dementia, as indicated in the Whitehall II cohort study (42). Thirdly, use of linkages to electronic health records may be high in specificity but low in sensitivity; moreover, without linkage to primary care data in our study, milder cases of dementia may have been missed (43). The percentage of AD out of all-cause dementia cases was low in our study (35%) compared with the report of the WHO (50–70%) (2); it is possible that some cases had not been clinically classified by type of dementia, which may attenuate associations between meat consumption and risk of AD. In addition, taking dates of hospital admission and death registry as proxy of diagnosis dates of incident dementia could have resulted in measurement errors; some incident cases might actually be prevalent cases diagnosed prior to hospital admission. Therefore, electronic linkages to accurate primary-care data should be taken into consideration for dementia ascertainment in future research.

Our findings suggest that consumption of processed meat may increase risk of incident dementia, and unprocessed red meat intake may be associated with lower risks, independent of APOE ε4 carriage. On the basis of the findings of this study, more specific public health guidance could be indicated differentiating between types of meat. However further research is recommended to confirm these results. Overall, the research adds to the growing body of evidence linking meat, especially processed meat consumption, to increased risk of a range of noncommunicable diseases.

Supplementary Material

nqab028_Supplemental_File

Acknowledgements

This study has been conducted using resources from UK Biobank under application number 48684. The authors thank Mary Mitchell as the PPI representative who has commented on our manuscript, Chunxiao Li (MRC Epidemiology Unit, University of Cambridge) for her advice on analyses of genetic data, and Timothy J Key and AuroraPerez-Cornago (University of Oxford) for their help with the standard portion sizes in the Oxford WebQ.

The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—HZ: conceived the study; HG, DG, LH, JC: worked on the study design, data acquisition, and statistical analyses; HZ: wrote the first draft of the manuscript and had primary responsibility for final content; DG, HR, DB, LH, JC: provided critical comments on the scientific interpretation of the results; all authors: made substantial contributions to revision of the manuscript; and all authors: read and approved the final manuscript.

Author disclosure: J Cade is the director of University of Leeds company Dietary Assessment Ltd. Other authors declare no competing interests. All other authors report no conflicts of interest.

Notes

This work was supported by the China Scholarship Council and the University of Leeds to HZ (201806010423). The sponsors had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of this manuscript.

Supplemental Methods 1–3, Supplemental Tables 1–9, and Supplemental Figures 1–10 are available from the “Supplementary data” link in the online posting of the article and from the same link in the online table of contents at https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/.

Abbreviations used: AD, Alzheimer disease; APOE, apolipoprotein E; ICD, International Classification of Diseases; VD, vascular dementia.

Contributor Information

Huifeng Zhang, Nutritional Epidemiology Group, School of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK.

Darren C Greenwood, Leeds Institute for Data Analytics, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK.

Harvey A Risch, Department of Chronic Disease Epidemiology, Yale School of Public Health, New Haven CT, USA.

David Bunce, School of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK.

Laura J Hardie, Division of Clinical and Population Sciences, Leeds Institute of Cardiovascular and Metabolic Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK.

Janet E Cade, Nutritional Epidemiology Group, School of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK.

Data Availability

The data sets described in the manuscript are not publicly available because the UK Biobank has proprietary rights of the data. External investigators can request the data and approval of use on application to the UK Biobank (www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/).

References

1. Larson EB, Yaffe K, Langa KM. New insights into the dementia epidemic. N Engl J Med. 2013;369:2275–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. World Health Organization. Dementia fact Sheet. 2019; [Internet]. Available from: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/en/ (accessed October 10 2019). [Google Scholar]

3. Burns A, Iliffe S. Dementia. BMJ. 2009;338:b75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Dauncey MJ. Nutrition, the brain and cognitive decline: insights from epigenetics. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2014;68:1179–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Lahiri DK, Maloney B, Basha MR, Ge YW, Zawia NH. How and when environmental agents and dietary factors affect the course of Alzheimer disease: the “LEARn” model (latent early-life associated regulation) may explain the triggering of AD. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2007;4:219–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Kivipelto M, Mangialasche F, Ngandu T. Lifestyle interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2018;14:653–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Bouvard V, Loomis D, Guyton KZ, Grosse Y, Ghissassi FE, Benbrahim-Tallaa L, Guha N, Mattock H, Straif K. Carcinogenicity of consumption of red and processed meat. Lancet Oncol. 2015;16:1599–600. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. World Agriculture: Towards 2015/2030, an FAO Perspective. 2015; [Internet]. Available from: http://www.fao.org/3/y4252e/y4252e05b.htm#P3_3 (accessed 05 April 2020). [Google Scholar]

9. Albanese E, Dangour AD, Uauy R, Acosta D, Guerra M, Guerra SS, Huang Y, Jacob KS, deRodriguez JL, Noriega LHet al.. Dietary fish and meat intake and dementia in Latin America, China, and India: a 10/66 Dementia Research Group population-based study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90:392–400. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Grant WB. Trends in diet and Alzheimer disease during the nutrition transition in Japan and developing countries. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;38:611–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Titova OE, Ax E, Brooks SJ, Sjögren P, Cederholm T, Kilander L, Kullberg J, Larsson EM, Johansson L, Ahlström Het al.. Mediterranean diet habits in older individuals: associations with cognitive functioning and brain volumes. Exp Gerontol. 2013;48:1443–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Zhang H, Hardie L, Bawajeeh AO, Cade J. Meat consumption, cognitive function and disorders: a systematic review with narrative synthesis and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2020;12:1528. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

13. Qian J, Wolters FJ, Beiser A, Haan M, Ikram MA, Karlawish J, Langbaum JB, Neuhaus JM, Reiman EM, Roberts JSet al.. APOE-related risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia for prevention trials: An analysis of four cohorts. PLoS Med. 2017;14:e1002254. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Licher S, Ahmad S, Karamujić-Čomić H, Voortman T, Leening MJG, Ikram MA, Ikram MK. Genetic predisposition, modifiable-risk-factor profile and long-term dementia risk in the general population. Nat Med. 2019;25:1364–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, Beral V, Burton P, Danesh J, Downey P, Elliott P, Green J, Landray Met al.. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Med. 2015;12:e1001779. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Bradbury KE, Young HJ, Guo W, Key TJ. Dietary assessment in UK Biobank: an evaluation of the performance of the touchscreen dietary questionnaire. J Nutr Sci. 2018;7:e6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Galante J, Adamska L, Young A, Young H, Littlejohns TJ, Gallacher J, Allen N. The acceptability of repeat Internet-based hybrid diet assessment of previous 24-h dietary intake: administration of the Oxford WebQ in UK Biobank. Br J Nutr. 2016;115:681–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Carter JL, Lewington S, Piernas C, Bradbury K, Key TJ, Jebb SA, Arnold M, Bennett D, Clarke R. Reproducibility of dietary intakes of macronutrients, specific food groups, and dietary patterns in 211 050 adults in the UK Biobank study. J Nutr Sci. 2019;8:e34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Bradbury KE, Tong TYN, Key TJ. Dietary intake of high-protein foods and other major foods in meat-eaters, poultry-eaters, fish-eaters, vegetarians, and vegans in UK biobank. Nutrients. 2017;9:1317. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Bradbury KE, Murphy N, Key TJ. Diet and colorectal cancer in UK Biobank: a prospective study. Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49:246–58. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. MacMahon S, Peto R, Cutler J, Collins R, Sorlie P, Neaton J, Abbott R, Godwin J, Dyer A, Stamler J. Blood pressure, stroke, and coronary heart disease. Part 1, prolonged differences in blood pressure: prospective observational studies corrected for the regression dilution bias. Lancet North Am Ed. 1990;335:765–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Bycroft C, Freeman C, Petkova D, Band G, Elliott LT, Sharp K, Motyer A, Vukcevic D, Delaneau O, O'Connell Jet al.. The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature. 2018;562:203–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Rutten-Jacobs LC, Larsson SC, Malik R, Rannikmäe K, Sudlow CL, Dichgans M, Markus HS, Traylor M. Genetic risk, incident stroke, and the benefits of adhering to a healthy lifestyle: cohort study of 306,473 UK Biobank participants. BMJ. 2018;363:k4168. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Bowman K, Jones L, Pilling LC, Delgado J, Kuchel GA, Ferrucci L, Fortinsky RH, Melzer D. Vitamin D levels and risk of delirium: a mendelian randomization study in the UK Biobank. Neurology. 2019;92:e1387–e94. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Power MC, Weuve J, Sharrett AR, Blacker D, Gottesman RF. Statins, cognition, and dementia—systematic review and methodological commentary. Nat Rev Neurol. 2015;11:220–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. VanderWeele TJ, Hernán MA, Robins JM. Causal directed acyclic graphs and the direction of unmeasured confounding bias. Epidemiology. 2008;19:720–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Lourida I, Hannon E, Littlejohns TJ, Langa KM, Hypponen E, Kuzma E, Llewellyn DJ. Association of lifestyle and genetic risk with incidence of dementia. JAMA. 2019;322:430–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Ngabirano L, Samieri C, Feart C, Gabelle A, Artero S, Duflos C, Berr C, Mura T. Intake of meat, fish, fruits, and vegetables and long-term risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;68:711–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Barberger-Gateau P, Letenneur L, Deschamps V, Pérès K, Dartigues JF, Renaud S. Fish, meat, and risk of dementia: cohort study. BMJ. 2002;325:932–3. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Fischer K, vanLent DM, Wolfsgruber S, Weinhold L, Kleineidam L, Bickel H, Scherer M, Eisele M, vandenBussche H, Wiese Bet al.. Prospective associations between single foods, Alzheimer's dementia and memory decline in the elderly. Nutrients. 2018;10:852. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Barberger-Gateau P, Raffaitin C, Letenneur L, Berr C, Tzourio C, Dartigues JF, Alpérovitch A. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three-City cohort study. Neurology. 2007;69:1921–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Huang TL, Zandi PP, Tucker KL, Fitzpatrick AL, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Burke GL, Carlson MC. Benefits of fatty fish on dementia risk are stronger for those without APOE epsilon4. Neurology. 2005;65:1409–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

33. Laitinen MH, Ngandu T, Rovio S, Helkala EL, Uusitalo U, Viitanen M, Nissinen A, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Kivipelto M. Fat intake at midlife and risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease: a population-based study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:99–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Kivipelto M, Rovio S, Ngandu T, Kåreholt I, Eskelinen M, Winblad B, Hachinski V, Cedazo-Minguez A, Soininen H, Tuomilehto Jet al.. Apolipoprotein E ɛ4 magnifies lifestyle risks for dementia: a population-based study. J Cell Mol Med. 2008;12:2762–71. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Ylilauri MP, Voutilainen S, Lönnroos E, Mursu J, Virtanen HE, Koskinen TT, Salonen JT, Tuomainen T-P, Virtanen JK. Association of dietary cholesterol and egg intakes with the risk of incident dementia or Alzheimer disease: the Kuopio Ischaemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;105:476–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Roberts RO, Roberts LA, Geda YE, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, O'Connor HM, Knopman DS, Petersen RC. Relative intake of macronutrients impacts risk of mild cognitive impairment or dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;32:329–39. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. Piñero DJ, Connor JR. Iron in the brain: an important contributor in normal and diseased states. Neuroscientist. 2000;6:435–53. [Google Scholar]

38. Liu J-L, Fan Y-G, Yang Z-S, Wang Z-Y, Guo C. Iron and Alzheimer's disease: from pathogenesis to therapeutic implications. Frontiers Neurosci. 2018;12;632. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

39. delaMonte SM, Neusner A, Chu J, Lawton M. Epidemiological trends strongly suggest exposures as etiologic agents in the pathogenesis of sporadic Alzheimer's disease, diabetes mellitus, and non-alcoholic steatohepatitis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;17:519–29. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Barnard ND, Bunner AE, Agarwal U. Saturated and trans fats and dementia: a systematic review. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35:S65–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

41. Guo C-P, Wei Z, Huang F, Qin M, Li X, Wang Y-M, Wang Q, Wang J-Z, Liu R, Zhang Bet al.. High salt induced hypertension leads to cognitive defect. Oncotarget. 2017;8:95780–90. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Sabia S, Dugravot A, Dartigues JF, Abell J, Elbaz A, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A. Physical activity, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia: 28 year follow-up of Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ. 2017;357:j2709. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Wilkinson T, Schnier C, Bush K, Rannikmäe K, Henshall DE, Lerpiniere C, Allen NE, Flaig R, Russ TC, Bathgate Det al.. Identifying dementia outcomes in UK Biobank: a validation study of primary care, hospital admissions and mortality data. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34:557–65. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition are provided here courtesy of American Society for Nutrition

Meat consumption and risk of incident dementia: cohort study of 493,888 UK Biobank participants (2024)
Top Articles
Polenta Fries (Gluten-Free, Vegan Recipe) - Elavegan
37 Recipes for Old-World Italian Food
My Arkansas Copa
Ups Stores Near
Kokichi's Day At The Zoo
Lifebridge Healthstream
Txtvrfy Sheridan Wy
877-668-5260 | 18776685260 - Robocaller Warning!
Moviesda Dubbed Tamil Movies
Unraveling The Mystery: Does Breckie Hill Have A Boyfriend?
Fallout 4 Pipboy Upgrades
South Ms Farm Trader
Lantana Blocc Compton Crips
Jcpenney At Home Associate Kiosk
Richmond Va Craigslist Com
Purple Crip Strain Leafly
735 Reeds Avenue 737 & 739 Reeds Ave., Red Bluff, CA 96080 - MLS# 20240686 | CENTURY 21
Craigslist Malone New York
Eva Mastromatteo Erie Pa
Roster Resource Orioles
Georgia Vehicle Registration Fees Calculator
How Much Is Tay Ks Bail
Christina Steele And Nathaniel Hadley Novel
Ahn Waterworks Urgent Care
Yosemite Sam Hood Ornament
Www Va Lottery Com Result
Valic Eremit
Hannah Palmer Listal
Craigslist Panama City Beach Fl Pets
Https E22 Ultipro Com Login Aspx
Spiritual Meaning Of Snake Tattoo: Healing And Rebirth!
Nk 1399
Lilpeachbutt69 Stephanie Chavez
Paradise Point Animal Hospital With Veterinarians On-The-Go
Funky Town Gore Cartel Video
Bratislava | Location, Map, History, Culture, & Facts
Why The Boogeyman Is Rated PG-13
R&J Travel And Tours Calendar
Ljw Obits
Greater Keene Men's Softball
Stanford Medicine scientists pinpoint COVID-19 virus’s entry and exit ports inside our noses
Restored Republic May 14 2023
Rhode Island High School Sports News & Headlines| Providence Journal
Flipper Zero Delivery Time
Chathuram Movie Download
Sig Mlok Bayonet Mount
Holzer Athena Portal
Online TikTok Voice Generator | Accurate & Realistic
Westport gun shops close after confusion over governor's 'essential' business list
Rocket Bot Royale Unblocked Games 66
Bluebird Valuation Appraiser Login
Fetllife Com
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Prof. An Powlowski

Last Updated:

Views: 5457

Rating: 4.3 / 5 (64 voted)

Reviews: 95% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Prof. An Powlowski

Birthday: 1992-09-29

Address: Apt. 994 8891 Orval Hill, Brittnyburgh, AZ 41023-0398

Phone: +26417467956738

Job: District Marketing Strategist

Hobby: Embroidery, Bodybuilding, Motor sports, Amateur radio, Wood carving, Whittling, Air sports

Introduction: My name is Prof. An Powlowski, I am a charming, helpful, attractive, good, graceful, thoughtful, vast person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.